1. Po raz pierwszy odwiedzasz EDU. LEARN

    Odwiedzasz EDU.LEARN

    Najlepszym sposobem na naukę języka jest jego używanie. W EDU.LEARN znajdziesz interesujące teksty i videa, które dadzą Ci taką właśnie możliwość. Nie przejmuj się - nasze filmiki mają napisy, dzięki którym lepiej je zrozumiesz. Dodatkowo, po kliknięciu na każde słówko, otrzymasz jego tłumaczenie oraz prawidłową wymowę.

    Nie, dziękuję
  2. Mini lekcje

    Podczas nauki języka bardzo ważny jest kontekst. Zdjęcia, przykłady użycia, dialogi, nagrania dźwiękowe - wszystko to pomaga Ci zrozumieć i zapamiętać nowe słowa i wyrażenia. Dlatego stworzyliśmy Mini lekcje. Są to krótkie lekcje, zawierające kontekstowe slajdy, które zwiększą efektywność Twojej nauki. Są cztery typy Mini lekcji - Gramatyka, Dialogi, Słówka i Obrazki.

    Dalej
  3. Wideo

    Ćwicz język obcy oglądając ciekawe filmiki. Wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na filmik. Nie martw się, obok każdego z nich są napisy. A może wcale nie będą Ci one potrzebne? Spróbuj!

    Dalej
  4. Teksty

    Czytaj ciekawe artykuły, z których nauczysz się nowych słówek i dowiesz więcej o rzeczach, które Cię interesują. Podobnie jak z filmikami, możesz wybrać temat oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknąć na wybrany artykuł. Nasz interaktywny słownik pomoże Ci zrozumieć nawet trudne teksty, a kontekst ułatwi zapamiętanie słówek. Dodatkowo, każdy artykuł może być przeczytany na głos przez wirtualnego lektora, dzięki czemu ćwiczysz słuchanie i wymowę!

    Dalej
  5. Słowa

    Tutaj możesz znaleźć swoją listę "Moje słówka", czyli funkcję wyszukiwania słówek - a wkrótce także słownik tematyczny. Do listy "Moje słówka" możesz dodawać słowa z sekcji Videa i Teksty. Każde z słówek dodanych do listy możesz powtórzyć później w jednym z naszych ćwiczeń. Dodatkowo, zawsze możesz iść do swojej listy i sprawdzić znaczenie, wymowę oraz użycie słówka w zdaniu. Użyj naszej wyszukiwarki słówek w części "Słownictwo", aby znaleźć słowa w naszej bazie.

    Dalej
  6. Lista tekstów

    Ta lista tekstów pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Teksty". Wybierz poziom trudności oraz temat, a następnie artykuł, który Cię interesuje. Kiedy już zostaniesz do niego przekierowany, kliknij na "Play", jeśli chcesz, aby został on odczytany przez wirtualnego lektora. W ten sposób ćwiczysz umiejętność słuchania. Niektóre z tekstów są szczególnie interesujące - mają one odznakę w prawym górnym rogu. Koniecznie je przeczytaj!

    Dalej
  7. Lista Video

    Ta lista filmików pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Video". Podobnie jak w przypadku Tekstów, najpierw wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na wybrane video. Te z odznaką w prawym górnym rogu są szczególnie interesujące - nie przegap ich!

    Dalej
  8. Dziękujemy za skorzystanie z przewodnika!

    Teraz już znasz wszystkie funkcje EDU.LEARN! Przygotowaliśmy do Ciebie wiele artykułów, filmików oraz mini lekcji - na pewno znajdziesz coś, co Cię zainteresuje!

    Teraz zapraszamy Cię do zarejestrowania się i odkrycia wszystkich możliwości portalu.

    Dziękuję, wrócę później
  9. Lista Pomocy

    Potrzebujesz z czymś pomocy? Sprawdź naszą listę poniżej:
    Nie, dziękuję

Już 62 409 użytkowników uczy się języków obcych z Edustation.

Możesz zarejestrować się już dziś i odebrać bonus w postaci 10 monet.

Jeżeli chcesz się dowiedzieć więcej o naszym portalu - kliknij tutaj

Jeszcze nie teraz

lub

Poziom:

Wszystkie

Nie masz konta?

7. Evolution, Ecology and Behavior: The Importance of Development in Evolution


Poziom:

Temat: Nauka i technologia

Prof: Today we're going to talk about,
or we're going to introduce the role of development in
evolution, and I would like to start by
asking you to make two jumps in your head.
In the first case I want you to think of yourself as inside a
single-celled bacterium.
If you go to a good book on the gene networks and the
biochemical networks that can be found inside a single-celled
bacterium, you will be stunned by the
complexity of it and you will deeply hope that you never have
to reproduce it on any examination,
because there is so much of it.
Okay?
That's inside one cell.
Development doesn't arise until we get to multi-cellular
organisms.
And if you were able to go into the body of a multi-cellular
organism, such as yourself,
and look at all the signaling pathways,
all the way that information is transferred and integrated in a
multi-cellular organism, it's just as complex as the
whole picture down inside that one single-celled bacterium.
So there are two huge kinds of orders of magnitude,
levels of hierarchical complexity of information
integration that happen in a multi-cellular organism like
yourself.
We call the way that the information in the genes maps
into the structure of the genotype the genotype-phenotype
map.
It takes us through all of that complexity to produce something
that we can then try to understand, which is a whole
organism.
Another name for the genotype-phenotype map is
development.
Okay?
And what I'm going to try to show you today is that out of
this almost unimaginable complexity,
two hierarchical layers of orders of magnitude of
information, biologists have been able to
extract some interesting simple rules and show that there are
some large-scale patterns in evolution.
However, that task is far from done,
and the understanding of the genotype-phenotype map,
or the understanding in general of developmental biology,
remains probably the most pressing issue in basic research
in biology for the twenty-first century.
It's something that occupies some of the best faculty in this
department and some of the best scientists across the globe.
So it's a basic issue.
And the thing that I hope you take home from this lecture
today is that it's important, both for microevolution,
and for macroevolution.
Today I will be talking about patterns that are pretty large
scale, they are more macroevolutionary;
and next time I will be talking about patterns that arise within
populations that reflect that macroevolutionary history,
but that have immediate consequences for microevolution.
So development isn't simple.
It's going on both at large scale, over long periods of
time, producing patterns,
and it's going on at a very, very short scale in every
generation, as each individual grows up and
turns into an adult.
So what's involved in development?
It isn't really just the production of the adult form
from an egg.
It is the living of the entire lifecycle,
from the formation of the gamete, through the adult,
through all the changes the adult goes through until it dies
and produces the next generation.
So development refers to the entire lifecycle,
and evolution shapes the entire lifecycle.
So something that we get from a very important discovery in
nineteenth century biology is that all of life is made of
cells.
It could've been organized differently,
and in fact there are interesting science fiction
novels, like Solaris,
by the great Polish science fiction novelist Stanislaw Lem,
that conceptualizes what life would be like if it were not
cellular.
For example, what if the whole ocean were
one living thing?
But we know that that's not the way life is on our planet.
On our planet life is all built out of cells,
and that means that the problem of development is a problem of
communicating between cells.
And cells are all set up as information signalers and
receivers.
They have cell adhesion molecules on their surfaces.
They produce information molecules, hormones,
and other signaling molecules for export.
This information is used to change the fate of a cell.
Now every cell in your body has all the information in it that's
needed to build you, and that is true of virtually
every organism.
The only exception in us is our red blood cells because they
don't have any nuclei, so they don't have any DNA in
them.
Okay?
But in every other organism that we know of,
with minor exceptions like our red blood cells,
all the information is in all the cells,
and that means that development is a matter of editing,
it's a matter of determining which information gets turned on
in the right place at the right time.
So the evolution of development is about shaping those patterns
in space and time, within the framework of an
organism, to produce something that works.
Development does a bunch of things in evolution.
One of the important ones is that it has a strong role in the
course of the production of individual organisms in shaping
the kind of variation that is presented to selection.
Okay?
So the developmental mechanisms that are shared by particular
organisms determine that only certain kinds of phenotypes are
going to be produced-- and there's a lot of variation
within those phenotypes-- but they are a tiny portion of
phenotype space.
And this is why we actually see, morphologically,
the Tree of Life.
This is why dogs look like wolves.
This is why humans look like chimpanzees.
This is why birds look like birds and we can call them
birds.
It's because they share developmental pathways that have
been inherited from ancestors and that have constrained the
range of phenotypes that can be presented to selection.
Now there's another important thing about development.
You might think that you could conceive of the body as produced
by an engineer, but it's not really constructed
that way in evolution.
What goes on is that genes can only build organisms out of the
materials that are available at a certain time,
and then there is an evolutionary memory,
of which materials are selected, and of the control
systems that are used to shape the phenotype with them.
Now I'll give you a couple of examples.
What is the cell membrane?
The cell membrane is a lipid bilayer, and it's been--it's
actually a marvelous organ now; it has all sorts of special
channels in it, things that are filters to let
particular stuff in and keep other stuff out.
It's been heavily modified by evolution.
But there is no way that you can take simply the DNA sequence
in the genome and get a reaction system that's going to construct
cell membranes.
All known cell membranes are actually constructed
biologically by using pre-existing cell membranes as
templates.
In other words, the cell membrane itself is an
information transfer molecule.
So that's one.
There's another, and that's bones.
Your bones are made out of a material called hydroxyapatite,
which is a calcium phosphate material.
And hydroxyapatite has the following extremely convenient
feature.
If you take hydroxyapatite and you put it under stress,
it will strengthen itself in the direction of the stress.
That means that the genes don't have to have sensor systems to
detect stress, and they don't have to worry
about how they're going to make a bone strong in the direction
of stress.
All they have to do is say, "Hey,
I'm going to use hydroxyapatite to make bones,
and then when that baby first starts to toddle around and walk
on its legs, its hips start getting
strengthened in the direction of stress."
Now, in fact, there are modifier genes that
then take this and use it to their own advantage,
by building in protein molecules that strengthen the
bone in the direction of the stress.
Okay?
But the initial signal, which direction is the stress
coming from, is a freebie.
It's given by the biochemical properties of a hydroxyapatite.
So that's one, a second one.
Here's a third one, gastrulation.
When vertebrate embryos, and many other embryos,
grow, they grow up as a ball of cells which then becomes a
hollow sphere of cells, and that hollow sphere of
cells, which by the time it gastrulates has thousands of
cells in it.
You can think of it as a little pulsing basketball.
Okay?
It's a little sphere that's pulsing,
and if you simply let this thing grow to a certain size,
the tension in the actin tubules in the cells will cause
it to spontaneously invaginate.
So it'll get a dimple in it, like you pushed your thumbs
into it.
This happens spontaneously.
It is not as though the genes have to say, "I am going to
construct a mechanism that's going to make my gastrula form.
When my blastula turns into a gastrula,
when my hollow ball of cells turns into a thing that's got a
dimple in it, and then forms three cell
layers, out of which I can make muscles and bones and skin and
gut and all of that kind of stuff,
when that happens that's a freebie."
Okay?
That is just in the tension of the actin filaments in an
expanding ball of cells.
So these are some of the properties of the biological
materials that organisms are constructed out of,
and it means that on the one hand the genes don't have
complete control over the phenotype,
but on the other hand they are given certain things by the
materials that don't have to be specified in the DNA sequence.
So where does development fit?
I'm giving you real large-scale messages now.
Okay?
The biological disciplines that we call Ecology and Behavior
actually deal with the processes that reduce the cohort of
newborn organisms to the ones that survive to reproduce.
Okay?
Ecology and Behavior actually study the mechanics of natural
selection, while they study a lot of other stuff as well.
But that's the level at which that happens.
Genetics takes the genotypes of the parents and it transforms
the genotypes of the parents through Hardy-Weinberg
equations, through the selection that's
operating on them, through all of that stuff that
we just looked at quickly.
It takes the genotypes of the parents and transforms them into
the genotypes of the offspring.
Okay?
So genetics is all about information transfer.
Lots of people that like computers do pretty well at
genetics.
What development does is it takes that information in the
genotype and it maps it into the material of the phenotype.
You can think of development as being a big transduction
mechanism that takes material--takes information and
turns it into material.
Okay?
And, in the process, it places limits on what the
phenotypes can look like, so that not every conceivable
phenotype is going to arise out of the DNA sequence in a genome;
only a certain range.
Flies are going to look like flies, sheep are going to look
like sheep, and daffodils are going to look like daffodils.
If we look at what development has been able to produce,
well.
in a large-scale it has produced some very basic things,
and we can see that by a comparison of the body plans of
the major groups of organisms.
I'd just like to take a moment here and see if I can elicit
from you some sense of what it is that we're looking at.
Okay?
I assume that you're all pretty comfortable with chordates,
because that's what you are.
Okay?
Can anybody tell me what a bryozoan is?
A bryozoan is a moss animalcule; it is a moss animal.
Bryozoans produce beautiful exoskeletons,
and you can find them on tropical reefs.
Anybody comfortable with what a priapulate is?
A priapulid is a deep-sea worm that forages with a tentacle and
when you pull it out of its hole, it looks like a penis;
which is why it's called a priapulid.
Okay? What about a tardigrade?
Tardigrades are little water bears.
Okay?
They look a little bit like insects or crustaceans,
and they're very tiny, and they're extremely cute.
Okay?
So tardigrades are water bears.
Arthropods, who's in the arthropods?
Insects are arthropods.
What are the other big groups of arthropods?
Student: >.
Prof: Crustaceans; yes spiders,
spiders and their relatives.
Arthropods are anything with jointed legs--that's just Greek
for jointed leg, arthro-pod.
Pogonophorans?
A tiny phylum of worms that live in the sand and are living
fossils and haven't really changed their morphology for
about 400 million years.
So there's a lot of big-scale stuff here.
This is basically the animal kingdom.
Okay?
And it's formed into these groups.
And just to give you a little bit of timeline,
it's about 600 million years, 700--Bilateria,
yeah, six or seven-hundred million years ago.
A lot of stuff happens really quickly, because from this point
to this point here is only about a hundred million years.
At this point we're still 500 million years ago.
Okay?
So this is big-scale stuff.
The cnidarians and the ctenophores formed trace fossils
in the pre-Cambrian ooze.
So they may go back a billion years;
not sure about how far back.
So if you look at what happened when multi-cellular organisms
formed and one branch of them went off to become animals,
this is what development was able to produce.
It could produce body axes--front and back,
left/right; produce a skeleton,
organ system, symmetry and cell layers.
And figuring out the shared general mechanisms by which
development can produce those things,
and then how evolution can tweak them to make them
different in these different groups,
is evolutionary developmental biology,
or evo-devo.
There are some big and striking differences among these groups.
For example, this group up here has an
exoskeleton, and this group down here has an endoskeleton;
and that places very fundamental constraints on
growth, size, all kinds of things.
What can it do in plants?
Well here is an extremely sketchy view of the plant world.
And, by the way, I would've used a much more
complex view of the plant world than this one,
but this is the one which is publicly okay without copyright
protection.
Okay?
So for you plant biologists that are worried about the view
of the plant world, this is pretty simple.
Basically what this does is it takes you from ferns and their
relatives through cycads, ginkgoes, pine trees and fir
trees; the gnetophyta,
which have some really cool plants that live in Namibia;
Wellitschia and other things like that are gnetophytes.
And then the magnoliophyta are all flowering plants,
going out here.
So this is a huge group down here.
And if you look at that, what has development and the
evolution of development been able to produce?
Well these guys are all variations on the following
themes: a meristem-root axis; where xylem and phloem appear;
where wood appears; the kinds of branching patterns
they have; whether they have naked or
covered seeds; whether they have leaves;
and whether they have flowers.
So you can see that the image that I'm trying to create here,
in both plants and in animals, is that there's certain shared
general features, and that what evolution has
done is that it has made many, many different combinations of
those features, to create the diversity that we
see, and that this is done through
the evolution of development.
Mechanistically a lot of this is going on at the level of gene
regulation; and this is a recap of the
structure of the eukaryotic gene.
And the parts that I want you to focus on right now are the
promoters and the enhancers.
So these are parts of the DNA molecule that receive a signal,
which says turn this gene on or turn it off.
And I want to show you just exactly what kind of a network
of information that results in.
You can think of there being regulators--
and we will talk a little bit about what kinds of regulators
are used early in development-- and that these then feed into a
signaling cascade where a signal goes out,
there's a receiver, there's a transducer that turns
that signal into a transcription factor.
A transcription factor then is going to go out and it's going
to bind to the enhancer region of a gene, or a promoter region
of a gene.
Okay, so it's bringing in a signal.
The signal might be either turn it on or turn it off,
but it's bringing in a signal; that's what a transcription
factor will do.
You can think of there being other pathways and other signals
going through them, and they can turn on
transcription factors, and some of those will come
down and sit down on the same gene.
It isn't all only one transcription factor that can
sit down on one gene.
In an average gene in Drosophila there are about ten
to twenty binding sites in its control region.
That means that ten to twenty different transcription factors
can sit down on a single gene, and one transcription factor
can bind to the control regions of anywhere from one gene to
several hundred genes.
Okay?
There are about 13,000 genes in a drosophila genome.
So that gives you some idea of the array of possibilities of
turning all those things on or off, as you go through.
That is a huge array of possibilities,
and I think you'll see, when I show you later,
how you take an onychophoran worm and turn it into a
Drosophila, that you could imagine
evolution might have had to use a lot of them,
in order to turn something like a worm into something like a
fruit fly.
There is another way to think about this.
Think about the control region of a gene as the keys on a
piano, and think about the
transcription factors as the fingers on the hand of the
person who's playing the piano, and think about evolution as
the composer who wrote the score.
You know perfectly well that you can play all kinds of tunes
on the keys of a piano, and you know perfectly well
that there are traditions in music of different kinds of
related music.
So anyone who has listened to Bach is probably going to
recognize Telemann, and people who have listened to
Schoenberg are going to recognize more modernist
composers.
So you can think of those as being clades,
and you can think of the cultural tradition as being
inherited, and there's constraining
variation within the range of scores that are composed on the
piano.
Of course, what we're dealing with in a genome is like the
biggest symphony orchestra you ever saw in your life.
Okay?
So it's much, much bigger than that.
So a few points about the control of development.
At the beginning of development, when the first cell
is getting set up to divide, and in the formation of the
very early multi-cellular stages,
there are concentration gradients that are produced.
So before the first cell divides, there will be like a
front end and a back end of the cell,
and chemistry will get set up to produce molecules that then
form a concentration gradient across the cell,
and the concentration of those molecules is positional
information on what's the front and what's the back.
And as the cell divides, it retains that information on
where it is in the front or the back.
That's how the Drosophila embryo is set up.
And we will also see that this kind of concentration gradient
is used in the construction of the vertebrate limb.
By the way, the signaling center on the vertebrate limb,
when it's just a little paddle of cells, is basically in the
armpit.
Okay?
So if you want to think of smelly molecules being produced,
think armpits.
What then happens is that transcription factors are used
to define specific areas where only a precise subset of genes
is expressed.
So remember, all the info in the whole
genome is in every cell.
You only want a certain subset for this part of the organism
that you're making.
So the gradient, the chemical gradient then sets
up gene expression, and the transcription factors
appropriate to that position get turned on.
Genes are regulated by combinations of activators and
repressors, and this combinatorial control
is what gives you the huge diversity of cell specific gene
expressions.
When I say combinatorial control, think of composers
writing notes and people playing pianos;
that's combinatorial control too.
All the music that's ever been written, that can be played on a
piano, is simply a variation on all the combinations of those
keys in space and time.
So, the control can get complex.
It can be a cascade of information, and genes that
produce transcription factors can be regulated by genes that
produce transcription factors.
And this sets up situations where genes can switch their
roles.
It is not correct to think that there are some genes that are
early development genes, and then there are other genes
that are adult genes.
Genes are used flexibly, in many different contexts,
depending upon the information that's coming in to regulate
them.
Certainly there are some genes that are quite important in the
embryo, but it turns out they also play a role in the adult.
We'll see a case of this a little bit later.
Not surprisingly it's the genes that determine the general
pattern that switch on first, and the ones that are
controlling detail switch on later.
So when I say 'general pattern' I mean--
for example, in the vertebrate embryo,
the front and the back, the top and the bottom,
the left and the right: that gets laid down first.
Then the embryo gets chopped up into a sequence of segments.
Some of them turn into the head, some of them turn into leg
segments; some of them have extremities,
some of them don't--that kind of thing.
Okay?
So this sequence of how the early general pattern gets set
up, and then how the details are
developed, that's all developmental
genetics, and that's produced in evolution a whole lot of stuff.
Now, a little bit of vocabulary.
You're going to hear, in this area,
about boxes.
Okay?
You're going to hear about homeoboxes, MADS boxes,
stuff like that.
I want to tell you what boxes are.
They are very highly conserved sequence motifs,
and they are found in the DNA that codes for a particular
family of transcription factors.
The reason that this sequence--by the way,
it's about I think--I think it's about--
I'm not sure whether its seventy to eighty codons,
or seventy to eighty nucleotides long.
But these boxes are not too long.
They're conserved because they have a very important function,
and that is to bind to the DNA.
So they have a helix twist, helix structure,
and that means that if the DNA molecule is here,
this part of that protein, this part of that transcription
factor, is going to fit right into it.
And it's because they are transcription factors--the boxes
are found in transcription factors--that this is a very
conserved interaction.
Because DNA hasn't changed its structure in three billion
years.
So if they're going to bind to it, they have to have that
structure, and so selection has made sure that that sequence is
preserved.
They're called boxes simply because if you lay these DNA
sequences out, if you sequence a lot of DNA,
and you are looking for one of these things,
what you find is that wherever there's a transcription factor,
there is a stereotypic sequence.
And the people who were analyzing this,
first on computer printouts, or now with imaging on computer
screens, drew boxes around them, to locate them.
That's why the word 'box.'
Okay?
So when you see one of these in a DNA sequence,
you know you probably have a gene for a transcription factor.
Here's the homeobox family.
Okay?
There are thirteen homeobox genes that have been identified.
And this is from a number of years ago;
this has probably been filled out now.
And they have two--well there's more than two striking things
about them.
But the first thing that's striking about them is that
they're deeply conserved.
That means that they have retained so much of their
sequence identity that you can recognize homeobox gene 1 in a
human, and you can see that the same
gene is there in flatworms and in earthworms,
in priapulids and so forth.
All the way through the animal kingdom this gene has been
conserved, and you can pick up something like it in cnidarian.
And you wonder, well how did there get to be
thirteen in this family?
Well here you can see a gene duplication event right here.
Homeobox gene 1 in the jellyfish and corals was
duplicated right here-- this was the expansion of the
central HOX genes-- and it happened here as well,
so that now there were two copies.
And that meant that this developmental control switch,
which was an extremely clever piece of machinery to have
around, now existed in two copies.
You could use the first one to do whatever it used to be doing,
and you can now evolve a new function for the second one.
This went on up until the time that the vertebrates started to
evolve.
And in our closest relatives there's one copy of the HOX
gene, and it was duplicated twice.
It was duplicated at the level of the Agnatha;
so the ancestors of the sharks.
And that means that all of the vertebrates, the higher
vertebrates, have four sets of developmental control genes.
Interestingly, the first set is still used to
lay down the major body axis, and the fourth set is used to
make a limb; that's the new function.
Okay?
Now they have deeply conserved sequences, but they are also
collinear.
By collinear, I mean this.
Look at the sequence on the genome, and look at what part of
the body is controlled.
The parts that are on one end control the head area,
the parts on the other end control the tail area,
and the parts in the middle control the stuff in the middle
of the body.
There isn't any logical reason why it had to be this way.
It's probably simply that when vertebrates first started--
well when animals, prior to vertebrates,
first started to get formed as multi-cellular things,
this happened to be one convenient way to control
development.
But logically speaking, giving the signaling apparatus
that's available in the genes, there's no reason logically
that the genes have to be collinear;
but it's a fact that they are, and it's a marvelous fact that
they are.
Okay?
This is just to show you that you can take homeobox genes and
look at their DNA sequences and say,
"Oh, they have similar DNA sequences."
And then look at what parts of the bodies they control and see
that a homeobox gene in a fly, that is homologous to a
homeobox gene in a mouse, is controlling a similar part
of the body.
So the things that are controlling the tail end,
the green genes here, are expressed in this part of
the fly and in this part of the mouse,
and the things that are controlling the head end,
that are expressed here in the fly,
are actually expressed here in the mouse.
That's kind of interesting, because it suggests that the
mouse has added on some stuff that is in front of the hind
brain.
So here's the vertebrate limb.
Okay?
And this is controlled by the fourth copy of the HOX genes,
the D copy.
And it shows you that if you have, for example,
D9 being the only one that's turned on, you get that.
If you have D9 and D10, you get that.
You have D9, D10, D11, you get that.
You have D9 through D12, you get that.
And you get all five of these on and you get that.
Okay?
So basically what this means is the following.
If you just have D9 on, make a shoulder;
D9 and D10, make a humerus; D9 through 11,
make a radius and ulna; D9 through D12, make a wrist;
and all five, make fingers.
How simple, how logical.
Remember when I said at the beginning we have these orders
of magnitude of complexity within cells,
and these orders of magnitude of complexity between cells.
Out of all of that complexity, this simple pattern emerges.
Oh, and I forgot, notice that the genes are
collinear in the limb.
Okay?
The ones that are on one end of the gene are controlling the
shoulder, and the ones that are on the other end of the gene are
controlling the fingers.
So it's still collinear.
It's the like the body axis, it's just been translated into
a limb.
Well what about flowers?
The MADS genes also have a sequence in them which shows
that they're a transcription factor;
there's a MADS box.
M-A-D-S is an acronym for the original names that these genes
had.
Okay?
Some of them started with an m, some with an a,
some with a d, some with an s.
Then after all that had happened, it was noticed that
they were related.
So people started calling them MADS genes.
They're scattered throughout the genome.
They are not collinear.
Okay?
In Arabidopsis, they're on all five
chromosomes.
There's nothing resembling the HOX genes in the way they're
genetically organized.
They fall into three groups: the A group,
the B group and the C group.
So the A is not a single gene, it's a group of related MADS
genes.
B is another group of related MADS genes.
And within each of these groups the genes are sharing
phylogenetic relationship.
That means that the members of the A group are probably all
duplicates of an ancestral gene; the members of the B group are
probably duplicates of an ancestral gene for the B group;
and so forth.
Now the neat thing about the MADS genes is the way they
control flowers.
And we're going to see that evolutionary developmental
biology has a lot to do with the production of beauty.
>
Two of the best understood examples in evolutionary
developmental biology are flowers and butterfly wings.
Okay?
So this is an area where researchers who go out to give
talks get to use a lot of neat slides.
The ABC model of flower development goes like this.
If only a gene from Group A is turned on, make a sepal;
if only A and B are turned on, make a petal;
if only B and C are turned on, make an anther;
if only C is turned on make a pistol and an ovary.
So the regulation of B and C is controlling male and female
organ development.
This is combinatorial.
Okay?
It's the same general logical principle.
However, it's with a completely different set of genes,
and plants evolved multi-cellularity independently
of animals, which means that plants
invented development in evolution independently of
animals.
They both hit upon combinatorial control as a
simple, logical way to control development.
That probably means it's a very good idea.
Okay?
It's a very simple and economical way of expressing
information.
Of course every gene has a history, and these MADS genes
were doing something before they made flowers.
In fact, if you go back and you look at the homologous genes in
the plants that do not have flowers,
you discover that in ferns they are controlling leaf
development, in conifers they are
controlling cone development; things like that.
It's not as though these genes were invented in order to make
flowers.
They were pre-existing, and they were co-opted by
evolution at the point where flowers started to evolve;
and gene duplications probably helped in that process.
So if we go back to the Burgess Shale, back to the Cambrian,
we find lots of onychophorans running around.
And if you go to an Australian rainforest today,
you will find onychophorans still running around,
and they look the same.
So 500 million years of evolution hasn't changed
onychophorans.
onychophorans are these neat, velvet worms.
They are, by the way, viviparous.
If you pick them up they squirt glue on you.
They have their own neat little biology.
They are the ancestors of the arthropods.
So what evolution basically did was it took an onychophoran and,
among other things, it turned them into fruit
flies, and butterflies,
and horseshoe crabs and king crabs and lobsters and shrimp.
How do you do it?
Well it was done basically by changing the range of segments
in which particular things were expressed.
If we go back here, you can see that the
onychophoran has a lot of segments.
It's got one leg on each segment.
Okay?
The fruit fly has many fewer segments and only six legs;
the onychophoran has fifty legs or so.
So basically what was going on is that the HOX genes were used
to say, "Oh, okay,
now we're going to say that these segments become a head,
these segments become a thorax, these segments become an
abdomen.
We're going to make antennae on the head,
and we're going to make wings on the thorax,
and we're going to make legs on the thorax,
and the abdomen isn't going to have any wings or legs."
Okay?
The way this initially happened--and you can find
fossils that replicate some of these stages--
you first make a generalized segment,
it's got both legs and wings on it.
Okay?
You make it many times.
So you've got both legs and wings on lots of segments.
Then you restrict the range of expression so that only certain
segments have wings, only certain segments have
legs.
You do that by altering the domain of expression of control
genes, and you use that kind of
combinatorial specificity to say,
"Okay, this is an antenna and not a leg.
Okay, it's an appendage growing out of the body wall,
but I'm going to make it into an antenna that's on this
segment and I'll make it into a leg if it's on that
segment."
So, of course, this goes on in seconds;
evolution took hundreds of millions of years.
So it's not the same process.
Some of these HOX genes have retained incredibly conserved
functions.
In a famous experiment that was done in the 1990s,
Walter Gehring's group in Switzerland,
took Pax6, which is a gene that's shared by all bilateral
organisms, and they genetically engineered
fruit flies with an extra copy of Pax6--
it is a gene that induces the development of eyes--
and by turning this gene on, they were able to make that
fruit fly grow eyes in unexpected places.
Okay?
So it could grow one on its--this is the regular eye;
this is an eye growing on an antenna;
this is an eye growing on a haltier;
and so forth.
The interesting thing was that they could do this with Pax6
from a human or a mouse; in other words,
the DNA sequence in the gene was so similar that it could be
used to control the developmental pathway in an
organism in which that gene had not been sitting for 600 million
years.
That's pretty remarkable.
Development is not easy to evolve, and I think this gets
across one of the reasons that it's not easy to evolve changes
in development.
Every organism has to function and reproduce in order for a
gene to get transmitted, and you can't tweak its
development around too much or you'll make it fall apart.
It's like you're driving down the road and you want to turn
your Volkswagen into a Mercedes Benz,
and so you get out your tools, and you're going sixty miles an
hour, and you want to modify it,
but you can't crash.
Okay?
So that causes constraints; there's only certain things
that you can do while you're moving down the road.
These developmental constraints are not permanent.
The genetic control of development does change more
slowly than many other things, but I would submit to you that
if we wiped out everything on the planet--
let's first duplicate earth ten million times,
and then let's go through and wipe out everything on all of
those ten million planets, except for one species,
and we leave it some food.
Okay?
It's the only thing that's there.
But on some planets all you've got is fruit flies.
On other planets all you've got is redwood trees.
On other planets all you've got is butterflies,
and on other planets you've got albatrosses;
but they have a food supply, they can live.
Give them long enough, go away in your spaceship,
come back, five billion, ten billion years later.
I would submit to you that every one of those planets is
going to have highly diverse life on it,
and that many of the things that we see on this planet you
will see on each one of those other planets.
They will contain a signature, probably a very interesting
signature, of this huge disturbance that has been
created on them.
But I think that it's possible for redwood trees to evolve into
squid.
I just think it takes them a very long time.
>
The things that change slowly constrain things that change
rapidly, and genes don't cause development by themselves.
They're steering the dynamics of gene products that interact
with environmental inputs.
So the genes actually are a fair distance away,
biochemically, physiologically,
from the things they're controlling.
They are working through complicated interaction systems.
So, a few take-home points.
Development maps the information in genotypes into
the material of phenotypes.
It's like the process that occurs when a blueprint that an
architect has drawn is turned into a building by the
construction company.
Developmental control genes use combinatorial logic.
There's a lot of other stuff that's really important in
evolutionary developmental biology, besides combinatorial
logic.
It happens to be a pet topic of mine.
You will find that if you talk to people who do this for their
profession, that they regard combinatorial
logic just as so natural, so much a part of the
landscape, that they hardly feel the need to mention it anymore.
But it is striking, when you compare plants and
animals, that they both hit upon this method of controlling gene
regulation.
The ancestors of currently existing organisms often had an
awful lot of the genes that are now involved in controlling
development.
Remember that phylogenetic tree I showed you for the HOX genes.
Many of them are present back in jellyfish,
and certainly many more of them are present in worms and
crustaceans and things like that.
So a lot of what has gone on in evolution is changing the
specificity of expression in time and space,
and the specificity of receptors in time and space.
Rather than necessarily evolving new genes that make new
kinds of proteins, a lot of evolution has been
concerned with making combinations of existing genes.
Interestingly, there is a very good
evolutionary developmental biologist at Wisconsin.
His name is Sean Carroll.
He is a charismatic guy.
And Sean has gone so far as to say that most of evolution,
in the last 500 million years, has consisted of the evolution
of gene regulation, rather than the evolution of
new structural proteins.
And, of course, by taking an extreme stance,
he has managed to generate a controversy in which people are
saying, "Oh no, it's not just that.
There are all of these other new structural proteins that are
going on."
And this is always very good for one's scientific career,
because both sides are increasing their publication
rates.
Now, in fact, both things have gone on.
Okay?
But by having a controversy, people get motivated to pin
down the details.
So I am making a little bit of fun of controversies,
but I also recognize that they are very strong motivating
forces.
So next time, we're going to talk about the
expression of genetic variation and reaction norms.
And I want you to remember today's lecture,
because today I have talked mostly about the big macro
picture; the impact and the patterns of
developmental mechanisms in the Tree of Life,
in all plants and animals.
And next time we're going to see what difference they make
within single populations and in the course of the lifetime of
single individuals.
This is one way that microevolution connects to
macroevolution; it connects through development.
Lunch is possible if you would like it.
Mobile Analytics